A decentralised system in systems theory is a system in which lower level components operate on local information to accomplish global goals. The global pattern of behaviour is an emergent property of dynamical mechanisms that act upon local components, such as indirect communication, rather than the result of a central ordering influence of a centralised system.

Graphical comparison of a centralised (A) and a decentralised (B) system

Centralised versus decentralised systems

edit

A centralised system is one in which a central controller exercises control over the lower-level components of the system directly or through the use of a power hierarchy (such as instructing a middle level component to instruct a lower level component).[1] The complex behaviour exhibited by this system is thus the result of the central controller's "control" over lower level components in the system, including the active supervision of the lower-level components.

A decentralised system, on the other hand, is one in which complex behaviour emerges through the work of lower level components operating on local information, not the instructions of any commanding influence. This form of control is known as distributed control, or control in which each component of the system is equally responsible for contributing to the global, complex behaviour by acting on local information in the appropriate manner. The lower level components are implicitly aware of these appropriate responses through mechanisms that are based on the component's interaction with the environment, including other components in that environment.

Self-organisation

edit

Decentralised systems are intricately linked to the idea of self-organisation—a phenomenon in which local interactions between components of a system establish order and coordination to achieve global goals without a central commanding influence. The rules specifying these interactions emerge from local information and in the case of biological (or biologically-inspired) agents, from the closely linked perception and action system of the agents.[2] These interactions continually form and depend on spatio-temporal patterns, which are created through the positive and negative feedback that the interactions provide. For example, recruitment in the foraging behaviour of ants relies on the positive feedback of the ant finding food at the end of a pheromone trail while ants' task-switching behaviour relies on the negative feedback of making antennal contact with a certain number of ants (for example, a sufficiently low encounter rate with successful foragers can cause a midden worker to switch to foraging, although other factors like food availability can affect the threshold for switching).

Examples

edit

While decentralised systems can easily be found in nature, they are also evident in aspects of human society such as governmental and economic systems.

Insect colonies

edit
 
Ants eating a piece of fruit

One of the most well known examples of a "natural" decentralized system is one used by certain insect colonies. In these insect colonies, control is distributed among the homogeneous biological agents who act upon local information and local interactions to collectively create complex, global behaviour. While individually exhibiting simple behaviours, these agents achieve global goals such as feeding the colony or raising the brood by using dynamical mechanisms like non-explicit communication and exploiting their closely coupled action and perception systems. Without any form of central control, these insect colonies achieve global goals by performing required tasks, responding to changing conditions in the colony environment in terms of task-activity, and subsequently adjusting the number of workers performing each task to ensure that all tasks are completed.[3] For example, ant colonies guide their global behaviour (in terms of foraging, patrolling, brood care, and nest maintenance) using a pulsing, shifting web of spatio-temporal patterned interactions that rely on antennal contact rate and olfactory sensing. While these interactions consist of both interactions with the environment and each other, ants do not direct the behaviour of other ants and thus never have a "central controller" dictating what is to be done to achieve global goals.

Instead, ants use a flexible task-allocation system that allows the colony to respond rapidly to changing needs for achieving these goals. This task-allocation system, similar to a division of labor is flexible in that all tasks rely on either the number of ant encounters (which take the form of antennal contact) and the sensing of chemical gradients (using olfactory sensing for pheromone trails) and can thus be applied to the entire ant population. While recent research has shown that certain tasks may have physiologically and age-based response thresholds,[4] all tasks can be completed by "any" ant in the colony.

For example, in foraging behaviour, red harvester ants (Pogonomyrmex barbatus) communicate to other ants where food is, how much food there is, and whether or not they should switch tasks to forage based on cuticular hydrocarbon scents and the rate of ant-interaction. By using the combined odors of forager cuticular hydrocarbons and of seeds[5] and interaction rate using brief antennal contact, the colony captures precise information about the current availability of food and thus whether or not they should switch to foraging behaviour "all without being directed by a central controller or even another ant". The rate at which foragers return with seeds sets the rate at which outgoing foragers leave the nest on foraging trips; faster rates of return indicate more food availability and fewer interactions indicate a greater need for foragers. A combination of these two factors, which are solely based on local information from the environment, leads to decisions about switching to the foraging task and ultimately, to achieving the global goal of feeding the colony.

In short, the use of a combination of simple cues makes it possible for red harvester ant colonies to make an accurate and rapid adjustment of foraging activity that corresponds to the current availability of food[6] while using positive feedback for regulation of the process: the faster outgoing foragers meet ants returning with seeds, the more ants go out to forage.[7] Ants then continue to use these local cues in finding food, as they use their olfactory senses to pick up pheromone trails laid by other ants and follow the trail in a descending gradient to the food source. Instead of being directed by other ants or being told as to where the food is, ants rely on their closely coupled action and perception systems to collectively complete the global task.[3]

While red harvester ant colonies achieve their global goals using a decentralised system, not all insect colonies function this way. For example, the foraging behaviour of wasps is under the constant regulation and control of the queen.[8]

The ant mill is an example of when a biological decentralized system fails, when the rules governing the individual agents are not sufficient to handle certain scenarios.

Human society: Market economy

edit

A market economy is an economy in which decisions on investment and the allocation of producer goods are mainly made through markets and not by a plan of production (see planned economy). A market economy is a decentralised economic system because it does not function via a central, economic plan (which is usually headed by a governmental body) but instead, acts through the distributed, local interactions in the market (e.g. individual investments). While a "market economy" is a broad term and can differ greatly in terms of state or governmental control (and thus central control), the final "behaviour" of any market economy emerges from these local interactions and is not directly the result of a central body's set of instructions or regulation.

Application

edit
 
Swarm of open-source Jasmine micro-robots recharging themselves

Artificial intelligence and robotics

edit

While classic artificial intelligence (AI) in the 1970s was focused on knowledge-based systems or planning robots, Rodney Brooks' behaviour-based robots and their success in acting in the real, unpredictably changing world has led many AI researchers to shift from a planned, centralised symbolic architecture to studying intelligence as an emergent product of simple interactions.[9] This thus reflects a general shift from applying a centralised system in robotics to applying a more decentralised system based on local interactions at various levels of abstraction.

For example, largely stemming from Newell and Simon's physical-symbol theory, researchers in the 1970s designed robots with a course of action that, when executed, would result in the achievement of some desired goal; thus the robots were seen as "intelligent" if they could follow the directions of their central controller (the program or the programmer) (for an example, see STRIPS). However, upon Rodney Brooks' introduction of subsumption architecture, which enabled robots to perform "intelligent" behaviour without using symbolic knowledge or explicit reasoning, increasingly more researchers have viewed intelligent behaviour as an emergent property that arises from an agent's interaction with the environment, including other agents in that environment.

While certain researchers have begun to design their robots with closely coupled perception and action systems and attempted to embody and situate their agents a la Brooks, other researchers have attempted to simulate multi-agent behaviour and thus further dissect the phenomena of decentralised systems in achieving global goals. For example, in 1996, Minar, Burkhard, Lang-ton and Askenazi created a multi-agent software platform for the stimulation of interacting agents and their emergent collective behaviour called "Swarm". While the basic unit in Swarm is the "swarm", a collection of agents executing a schedule of actions, agents can be composed of swarms of other agents in nested structures. As the software also provides object-oriented libraries of reusable components for building models and analyzing, displaying and controlling experiments on those models, it ultimately attempts to not only simulate multi-agent behaviour but to serve as a basis for further exploration of how collective groups of agents can achieve global goals through careful, yet implicit, coordination.[10]

See also

edit

Examples of decentralized systems:

References

edit
  1. ^ Bekey, George A. (2005). Autonomous Robots: From Biological Inspiration to Implementation and Control. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-02578-7.[page needed]
  2. ^ Bonabeau, Eric; Theraulaz, Guy; Deneubourg, Jean-Louls; Aron, Serge; Camazine, Scott (1997). "Self-organization in social insects" (PDF). Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 12 (5): 188–93. Bibcode:1997TEcoE..12..188B. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(97)01048-3. PMID 21238030.
  3. ^ a b Gordon, D. (2010). Ant Encounters: Interaction Networks and Colony Behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton U Press. [page needed]
  4. ^ Robinson, EJ; Feinerman, O; Franks, NR (2009). "Flexible task allocation and the organization of work in ants". Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 276 (1677): 4373–80. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1244. PMC 2817103. PMID 19776072.
  5. ^ Greene, Michael J.; Gordon, Deborah M. (2003). "Social insects: Cuticular hydrocarbons inform task decisions". Nature. 423 (6935): 32. Bibcode:2003Natur.423...32G. doi:10.1038/423032a. PMID 12721617. S2CID 4300832.
  6. ^ Greene, Michael J.; Pinter-Wollman, Noa; Gordon, Deborah M. (2013). Fenton, Brock (ed.). "Interactions with Combined Chemical Cues Inform Harvester Ant Foragers' Decisions to Leave the Nest in Search of Food". PLOS ONE. 8 (1): e52219. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...852219G. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052219. PMC 3540075. PMID 23308106.
  7. ^ Carey, Bjorn (May 15, 2013). "Evolution shapes new rules for ant behavior, Stanford research finds". Stanford Report. Retrieved November 21, 2013.
  8. ^ Reeve, Hudson K.; Gamboa, George J. (1987). "Queen Regulation of Worker Foraging in Paper Wasps: A Social Feedback Control System (Polistes Fuscatus, Hymenoptera: Vespidae)". Behaviour. 102 (3): 147. doi:10.1163/156853986X00090.
  9. ^ Brooks, R. (1986). "A robust layered control system for a mobile robot". IEEE Journal on Robotics and Automation. 2: 14–23. doi:10.1109/JRA.1986.1087032. hdl:1721.1/6432. S2CID 10542804. Archived from the original on September 22, 2017.
  10. ^ Minar, N.; Burkhart, R.; Langton, C.; Askenazi, M. (1996). "The Swarm Simulation System: A Toolkit for Building Multi-Agent Simulations". SFI Working Papers. Santa Fe Institute.

Further reading

edit
  • Camazine, Scott; Sneyd, James (1991). "A model of collective nectar source selection by honey bees: Self-organization through simple rules". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 149 (4): 547. Bibcode:1991JThBi.149..547C. doi:10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80098-0.
  • Kernis, Michael H.; Cornell, David P.; Sun, Chien-ru; Berry, Andrea; Harlow, T (1993). "There's more to self-esteem than whether it is high or low: The importance of stability of self-esteem". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 65 (6): 1190–204. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1190. PMID 8295118.
  • Miller, Peter (July 2007). "Swarm Theory". National Geographic. Archived from the original on May 19, 2008. Retrieved November 21, 2013.
  • Abeysinghe, Asanka (July 2018). "Cell-based Architecture". WSO2, Inc. Retrieved February 14, 2019.